banner



Does Animal Sleep In Any Particular Direction

Sleep in animals refers to a behavioral and physiological state characterized by altered consciousness, reduced responsiveness to external stimuli, and homeostatic regulation observed in various animals. Sleep has been observed in mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians, and some fish, and, in some class, in insects and even in simpler animals such as nematodes. The internal cyclic clock promotes sleep at nighttime for diurnal organisms (such as humans) and in the solar day for nocturnal organisms (such as rodents). Sleep patterns vary widely amidst species. It appears to be a requirement for all mammals and virtually other animals.

Definition [edit]

Sleep tin can follow a physiological or behavioral definition. In the physiological sense, sleep is a country characterized by reversible unconsciousness, special brainwave patterns, sporadic eye motion, loss of musculus tone (maybe with some exceptions; meet below regarding the sleep of birds and of aquatic mammals), and a compensatory increment following impecuniousness of the state, this last known as slumber homeostasis (i.due east., the longer a waking land lasts, the greater the intensity and duration of the slumber country thereafter).[1] [2] In the behavioral sense, sleep is characterized by minimal movement, non-responsiveness to external stimuli (i.eastward. increased sensory threshold), the adoption of a typical posture, and the occupation of a sheltered site, all of which is normally repeated on a 24-hour basis.[3] The physiological definition applies well to birds and mammals, but in other animals (whose brain is not equally complex), the behavioral definition is more than oftentimes used. In very unproblematic animals, behavioral definitions of slumber are the only ones possible, and even then the behavioral repertoire of the fauna may not be extensive enough to let distinction between sleep and wakefulness.[4] Sleep is quickly reversible, as opposed to hibernation or coma, and sleep deprivation is followed past longer or deeper rebound sleep.

Necessity [edit]

If slumber were not essential, one would expect to find:

  • Animal species that practice not sleep at all
  • Animals that practise not need recovery sleep later staying awake longer than usual
  • Animals that suffer no serious consequences as a result of lack of slumber

Outside of a few basal animals that take no encephalon or a very simple one, no animals take been found to appointment that satisfy any of these criteria.[5] While some varieties of shark, such as great whites and hammerheads, must remain in motility at all times to motility oxygenated water over their gills, information technology is possible they still slumber one cognitive hemisphere at a time as marine mammals practise. However it remains to exist shown definitively whether any fish is capable of unihemispheric sleep.[ commendation needed ]

Invertebrates [edit]

Sleep equally a phenomenon appears to have very old evolutionary roots. Unicellular organisms do non necessarily "sleep", although many of them take pronounced circadian rhythms. The fresh-h2o polyp Hydra vulgaris and the jellyfish Cassiopea are among the most primitive organisms in which sleep-like states have been observed.[half-dozen] [vii] Observing sleep states in jellyfish provides testify that sleep states practice not crave that an beast have a brain or central nervous system.[8] The nematode C. elegans is some other primitive organism that appears to require sleep. Hither, a lethargus phase occurs in brusque periods preceding each moult, a fact which may indicate that sleep primitively is connected to developmental processes. Raizen et al.'s results[9] furthermore advise that slumber is necessary for changes in the neural organization.

A cuckoo bee from the genus Nomada, sleeping. Annotation the characteristic position anchored by the mandibles. Bees accept some of the well-nigh complex sleep states amidst insects.[10]

Decade after decade results mounted that insects exercise slumber, and that this resembles mammalian and avian sleep. Nonetheless sleep scientists connected to not accept these results and there was wide understanding that insects did non experience slumber. It took the gene expression studies of Hendricks et al 2000 and Shaw et al 2000[xi] [12] showing orthology betwixt mammal and the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster for this to finally exist accepted. The electrophysiological study of sleep in pocket-size invertebrates is complicated. Insects go through circadian rhythms of activity and passivity but some practice not seem to accept a homeostatic slumber need. Insects do non seem to exhibit REM sleep. However, fruit flies announced to slumber, and systematic disturbance of that state leads to cerebral disabilities.[13] There are several methods of measuring cognitive functions in fruit flies. A common method is to let the flies choose whether they want to fly through a tunnel that leads to a light source, or through a dark tunnel. Normally, flies are attracted to light. But if carbohydrate is placed in the finish of the dark tunnel, and something the flies dislike is placed in the cease of the light tunnel, the flies will somewhen learn to fly towards darkness rather than lite. Flies deprived of sleep require a longer fourth dimension to learn this and also forget it more quickly. If an arthropod is experimentally kept awake longer than information technology is used to, then its coming residual menstruation will be prolonged. In cockroaches that rest period is characterized by the antennae existence folded down and past a decreased sensitivity to external stimuli.[14] Slumber has been described in crayfish, too, characterized past passivity and increased thresholds for sensory stimuli as well as changes in the EEG design, markedly differing from the patterns found in crayfish when they are awake.[15] In honeybees, it has been shown that they apply sleep to store long term memories.[16]

Fish [edit]

Alternating phases of slumber and action in an adult zebrafish

Sleep in fish is the subject of ongoing scientific research.[17] [18] Typically fish exhibit periods of inactivity just show no significant reactions to deprivation of this condition. Some species that ever live in shoals or that swim continuously (because of a need for ram ventilation of the gills, for example) are suspected never to sleep.[19] There is as well incertitude about certain blind species that live in caves.[20] Other fish seem to slumber, notwithstanding. For example, zebrafish,[21] [22] tilapia,[23] tench,[24] brown bullhead,[25] and swell shark[26] become motionless and unresponsive at night (or by twenty-four hours, in the case of the swell shark); Castilian hogfish and blue-headed wrasse tin can even be lifted by paw all the style to the surface without evoking a response.[27] A 1961 observational study of approximately 200 species in European public aquaria reported many cases of credible sleep.[28] On the other hand, slumber patterns are easily disrupted and may even disappear during periods of migration, spawning, and parental care.[29]

Land vertebrates [edit]

Mammals, birds and reptiles evolved from amniotic ancestors, the starting time vertebrates with life cycles contained of water. The fact that birds and mammals are the only known animals to showroom REM and NREM slumber indicates a mutual trait before departure.[30] Withal, contempo show of REM-like sleep in fish suggests this difference may have occurred much earlier than previously thought.[31] Up to this signal, reptiles were considered the about logical grouping to investigate the origins of slumber. Daytime activity in reptiles alternates between basking and short bouts of active behavior, which has pregnant neurological and physiological similarities to slumber states in mammals. Information technology is proposed that REM sleep evolved from short bouts of motor action in reptiles while Irksome-Moving ridge Sleep (SWS) evolved from their basking state which shows like slow moving ridge EEG patterns.[32]

Reptiles and amphibians [edit]

Reptiles have quiescent periods similar to mammalian sleep, and a decrease in electric activity in the brain has been registered when the animals have been asleep. However, the EEG pattern in reptilian slumber differs from what is seen in mammals and other animals.[4] In reptiles, sleep time increases post-obit sleep deprivation, and stronger stimuli are needed to awaken the animals when they have been deprived of sleep as compared to when they have slept normally. This suggests that the sleep which follows impecuniousness is compensatorily deeper.[33]

In 2016, a study[34] reported the existence of REM- and NREM-like sleep stages in the Australian dragon Pogona vitticeps. Amphibians have periods of inactivity only show high vigilance (receptivity to potentially threatening stimuli) in this state.

A flamingo with at least one cerebral hemisphere awake

Birds [edit]

In that location are pregnant similarities between sleep in birds and sleep in mammals,[35] which is one of the reasons for the idea that slumber in higher animals with its partition into REM and NREM slumber has evolved together with warm-bloodedness.[36] Birds compensate for sleep loss in a manner like to mammals, by deeper or more intense slow-wave slumber (SWS).[37]

Birds have both REM and NREM sleep, and the EEG patterns of both have similarities to those of mammals. Unlike birds slumber different amounts, only the associations seen in mammals between slumber and variables such every bit body mass, encephalon mass, relative brain mass, basal metabolism and other factors (run into beneath) are not institute in birds. The only articulate explanatory factor for the variations in sleep amounts for birds of dissimilar species is that birds who sleep in environments where they are exposed to predators have less deep sleep than birds sleeping in more protected environments.[38]

Birds practice non necessarily exhibit slumber debt, just a peculiarity that birds share with aquatic mammals, and mayhap also with sure species of lizards (opinions differ about that final point[ description needed ]), is the power for unihemispheric sleep. That is the ability to sleep with one cerebral hemisphere at a time, while the other hemisphere is awake (Unihemispheric wearisome-moving ridge slumber).[39] When only one hemisphere is sleeping, only the contralateral eye volition be close; that is, when the correct hemisphere is asleep the left eye will be shut, and vice versa.[xl] The distribution of sleep between the two hemispheres and the amount of unihemispheric sleep are determined both by which part of the brain has been the most agile during the previous menses of wake[41]—that part will slumber the deepest—and it is also determined by the take chances of attacks from predators. Ducks nigh the perimeter of the flock are probable to exist the ones that first will observe predator attacks. These ducks accept significantly more unihemispheric slumber than those who slumber in the middle of the flock, and they react to threatening stimuli seen past the open up eye.[42]

Opinions partly differ almost sleep in migratory birds.[ citation needed ] The controversy is mainly about whether they tin slumber while flying or not.[ commendation needed ] Theoretically, certain types of sleep could be possible while flying, but technical difficulties preclude the recording of encephalon activity in birds while they are flight.

Mammals [edit]

Mammals accept wide multifariousness in sleep phenomena. Generally, they go through periods of alternate non-REM and REM sleep, simply these manifest differently. Horses and other herbivorous ungulates can slumber while continuing, but must necessarily lie downwardly for REM slumber (which causes muscular atony) for curt periods. Giraffes, for case, but demand to prevarication down for REM sleep for a few minutes at a time. Bats sleep while hanging upside downwards. Male person armadillos get erections during non-REM slumber, and the inverse is true in rats.[43] Early mammals engaged in polyphasic sleep, dividing slumber into multiple bouts per solar day. Higher daily sleep quotas and shorter sleep cycles in polyphasic species as compared to monophasic species, propose that polyphasic sleep may be a less efficient ways of attaining sleep's benefits. Small species with higher basal metabolic charge per unit (BMR) may therefore accept less efficient sleep patterns. It follows that the evolution of monophasic sleep may hitherto exist an unknown reward of evolving larger mammalian body sizes and therefore lower BMR.[44]

Slumber is sometimes thought to help conserve energy, though this theory is non fully adequate equally it only decreases metabolism past about 5–10%.[45] [46] Additionally information technology is observed that mammals crave sleep even during the hypometabolic country of hibernation, in which circumstance it is actually a net loss of energy as the animal returns from hypothermia to euthermia in social club to sleep.[47]

Nocturnal animals have higher body temperatures, greater action, rising serotonin, and diminishing cortisol during the night—the inverse of diurnal animals. Nocturnal and diurnal animals both have increased electrical activity in the suprachiasmatic nucleus, and corresponding secretion of melatonin from the pineal gland, at night.[48] Nocturnal mammals, which tend to stay awake at night, accept higher melatonin at night just like diurnal mammals practice.[49] And, although removing the pineal gland in many animals abolishes melatonin rhythms, it does not stop circadian rhythms altogether—though information technology may alter them and weaken their responsiveness to light cues.[50] Cortisol levels in diurnal animals typically rise throughout the dark, peak in the awakening hours, and diminish during the day.[51] [52] In diurnal animals, sleepiness increases during the night.

Duration [edit]

Different mammals sleep unlike amounts. Some, such as bats, sleep 18–twenty hours per day, while others, including giraffes, sleep only 3–4 hours per day. In that location can be large differences even between closely related species. There can likewise be differences between laboratory and field studies: for example, researchers in 1983 reported that convict sloths slept virtually xvi hours a day, merely in 2008, when miniature neurophysiological recorders were adult that could be affixed to wildlife, sloths in nature were found to sleep only nine.half dozen hours a day.[53]

As with birds, the primary dominion for mammals (with certain exceptions, meet beneath) is that they take two essentially different stages of slumber: REM and NREM sleep (see above). Mammals' feeding habits are associated with their sleep length. The daily need for sleep is highest in carnivores, lower in omnivores and everyman in herbivores. Humans slumber less than many other omnivores but otherwise not unusually much or unusually petty in comparison with other mammals.[54]

Many herbivores, similar Ruminantia (such equally cattle), spend much of their wake time in a state of drowsiness,[ further explanation needed ] which maybe could partly explain their relatively depression need for sleep. In herbivores, an inverse correlation is apparent between body mass and sleep length; large mammals sleep less than smaller ones. This correlation is idea to explain almost 25% of the difference in sleep amount between unlike mammals.[54] Also, the length of a particular slumber cycle is associated with the size of the animal; on boilerplate, bigger animals will take sleep cycles of longer durations than smaller animals. Slumber corporeality is also coupled to factors similar basal metabolism, brain mass, and relative encephalon mass.[ citation needed ] The elapsing of slumber among species is likewise direct related to BMR. Rats, which have a high BMR, sleep for upward to 14 hours a day, whereas elephants and giraffes, which have lower BMRs, sleep merely two–4 hours per day.[55]

Information technology has been suggested that mammalian species which invest in longer sleep times are investing in the immune system, every bit species with the longer sleep times have higher white blood cell counts.[56] Mammals born with well-developed regulatory systems, such equally the equus caballus and giraffe, tend to take less REM slumber than the species which are less developed at nascence, such as cats and rats.[57] This appears to repeat the greater need for REM slumber among newborns than among adults in almost mammal species. Many mammals sleep for a big proportion of each 24-hr flow when they are very young.[58] The giraffe only sleeps 2 hours a twenty-four hours in well-nigh 5–xv minute sessions. Koalas are the longest sleeping-mammals, almost 20–22 hours a day. However, killer whales and another dolphins do non sleep during the first month of life.[59] Instead, immature dolphins and whales frequently take rests past pressing their body next to their female parent'southward while she swims. Equally the mother swims she is keeping her offspring adrift to prevent them from drowning. This allows young dolphins and whales to remainder, which will aid keep their immune system salubrious; in turn, protecting them from illnesses.[60] During this period, mothers often sacrifice slumber for the protection of their young from predators. All the same, unlike other mammals, adult dolphins and whales are able to go without sleep for a month.[60] [61]

Comparative average sleep periods for various mammals (in captivity) over 24 hours[ commendation needed ]

  • Horses – two hours[62]
  • Elephants – 3+ hours [55]
  • Cows – 4.0 hours
  • Giraffes – 4.5 hours
  • Humans – eight.0 hours
  • Rabbits – 8.4 hours
  • Chimpanzees – 9.7 hours
  • Ruby foxes – 9.8 hours
  • Dogs – x.1 hours
  • Tigers – 15.8 hours
  • House mice – 12.5 hours
  • Cats – 12.v hours
  • Lions – 13.five hours
  • Platypuses – 14 hours
  • Chipmunks – 15 hours
  • Giant armadillos – 18.i hours
  • Leopards – eighteen hours
  • Piffling brown bats – xix.9 hours

Reasons given for the wide variations include the fact that mammals "that nap in hiding, like bats or rodents tend to have longer, deeper snoozes than those on constant alarm." Lions, which have fiddling fear of predators also accept relatively long slumber periods, while elephants have to consume virtually of the fourth dimension to support their huge bodies. Little chocolate-brown bats conserve their energy except for the few hours each nighttime when their insect prey are available, and platypuses eat a high free energy crustacean diet and, therefore, probably do not demand to spend as much time awake as many other mammals.[63]

Rodents [edit]

A report conducted by Datta indirectly supports the idea that memory benefits from sleep.[64] A box was constructed wherein a single rat could move freely from ane terminate to the other. The lesser of the box was made of a steel grate. A light would smoothen in the box accompanied by a sound. After a v-second delay, an electrical shock would be applied. One time the daze commenced, the rat could move to the other end of the box, ending the daze immediately. The rat could besides use the v-2d filibuster to movement to the other stop of the box and avoid the stupor entirely. The length of the shock never exceeded v seconds. This was repeated thirty times for one-half the rats. The other half, the command grouping, was placed in the same trial, but the rats were shocked regardless of their reaction. Later each of the grooming sessions, the rat would be placed in a recording cage for six hours of polygraphic recordings. This process was repeated for three consecutive days. During the posttrial sleep recording session, rats spent 25.47% more time in REM sleep after learning trials than subsequently control trials.[64]

An ascertainment of the Datta study is that the learning grouping spent 180% more than time in SWS than did the control group during the mail service-trial slumber-recording session.[65] This study shows that after spatial exploration action, patterns of hippocampal identify cells are reactivated during SWS following the experiment. Rats were run through a linear track using rewards on either finish. The rats would then exist placed in the track for thirty minutes to allow them to adapt (PRE), and so they ran the track with reward-based training for 30 minutes (RUN), and then they were allowed to rest for 30 minutes.

During each of these three periods, EEG data were collected for information on the rats' sleep stages. The hateful firing rates of hippocampal place cells during prebehavior SWS (PRE) and iii ten-minute intervals in postbehavior SWS (POST) were calculated by averaging across 22 rail-running sessions from vii rats. The results showed that ten minutes later the trial RUN session, there was a 12% increase in the mean firing rate of hippocampal place cells from the PRE level. After 20 minutes, the hateful firing rate returned apace toward the PRE level. The elevated firing of hippocampal place cells during SWS afterward spatial exploration could explain why at that place were elevated levels of slow-moving ridge sleep in Datta'southward study, as it besides dealt with a form of spatial exploration.

In rats, sleep deprivation causes weight loss and reduced torso temperature. Rats kept awake indefinitely develop skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and, eventually, fatal sepsis.[66] Slumber deprivation as well hinders the healing of burns on rats.[67] When compared with a control grouping, sleep-deprived rats' claret tests indicated a 20% decrease in white blood cell count, a meaning change in the immune arrangement.[68]

In mice, slumber deprivation caused past predictable chronic mild stress, which led to a subtract in slow-wave activity, was found to be linked with increased hurting sensations or hyperalgesia.[69] A 2014 study institute that depriving mice of sleep increased cancer growth and dampened the immune system's ability to command cancers. The researchers constitute higher levels of M2 tumor-associated macrophages and TLR4 molecules in the sleep deprived mice and proposed this every bit the mechanism for increased susceptibility of the mice to cancer growth. M2 cells suppress the immune system and encourage tumour growth. TRL4 molecules are signalling molecules in the activation of the immune arrangement.[lxx]

Monotremes [edit]

A platypus sleeping in the water before dark

Since monotremes (egg-laying mammals) are considered to stand for one of the evolutionarily oldest groups of mammals, they accept been subject to special involvement in the report of mammalian sleep. As early on studies of these animals could non find clear evidence for REM sleep, it was initially causeless that such slumber did not exist in monotremes, simply developed after the monotremes branched off from the rest of the mammalian evolutionary line, and became a separate, distinct group. Even so, EEG recordings of the brain stem in monotremes show a firing pattern that is quite similar to the patterns seen in REM sleep in higher mammals.[71] [72] In fact, the largest amount of REM sleep known in any animal is found in the platypus.[73] REM electrical activation does non extend at all to the forebrain in platypods, suggesting that they do not dream. The average sleep time of the platypus in a 24-60 minutes period is said to exist as long as xiv hours, though this may be considering of their loftier-calorie crustacean nutrition.[63]

Aquatic mammals [edit]

The consequences of falling into a deep sleep for marine mammalian species tin exist suffocation and drowning, or becoming easy prey for predators. Thus, dolphins, whales, and pinnipeds (seals) appoint in unihemispheric slumber while pond, which allows one brain hemisphere to remain fully functional, while the other goes to sleep. The hemisphere that is asleep alternates, and then that both hemispheres tin exist fully rested.[threescore] [74] But like terrestrial mammals, pinnipeds that sleep on country autumn into a deep sleep and both hemispheres of their encephalon shut down and are in full slumber mode.[75] [76] Aquatic mammal infants practise not have REM sleep in infancy;[77] REM sleep increases as they age.

Among others, seals and whales belong to the aquatic mammals. Earless seals and eared seals have solved the problem of sleeping in water via two different methods. Eared seals, similar whales, show unihemispheric sleep. The sleeping one-half of the brain does not awaken when they surface to exhale. When i half of a seal's encephalon shows tiresome-wave sleep, the flippers and whiskers on its opposite side are immobile. While in the water, these seals take almost no REM sleep and may get a calendar week or two without it. As before long as they move onto land they switch to bilateral REM sleep and NREM sleep comparable to state mammals, surprising researchers with their lack of "recovery sleep" afterwards missing and so much REM.

Earless seals sleep bihemispherically similar nigh mammals, under water, hanging at the water surface or on land. They hold their breath while sleeping nether water, and wake up regularly to surface and exhale. They can too hang with their nostrils above water and in that position have REM sleep, simply they exercise not accept REM sleep underwater.

REM sleep has been observed in the airplane pilot whale, a species of dolphin.[78] Whales practise non seem to have REM sleep, nor do they seem to accept whatever problems because of this. One reason REM sleep might be difficult in marine settings is the fact that REM sleep causes muscular atony; that is to say, a functional paralysis of skeletal muscles that can be hard to combine with the need to breathe regularly.[54] [79]

Conscious animate cetaceans sleep simply cannot afford to be unconscious for long, because they may drown. While noesis of slumber in wild cetaceans is limited, toothed cetaceans in captivity have been recorded to exhibit unihemispheric deadening-wave sleep (USWS), which means they sleep with one side of their brain at a time, then that they may swim, exhale consciously and avert both predators and social contact during their menstruum of rest.[lxxx]

A 2008 study plant that sperm whales sleep in vertical postures merely nether the surface in passive shallow 'drift-dives', more often than not during the day, during which whales exercise not respond to passing vessels unless they are in contact, leading to the proffer that whales maybe slumber during such dives.[81]

Unihemispherism [edit]

Unihemispheric sleep refers to sleeping with only a single cerebral hemisphere. The phenomenon has been observed in birds and aquatic mammals,[82] as well equally in several reptilian species (the latter being disputed: many reptiles behave in a way which could be construed as unihemispheric sleeping, just EEG studies have given contradictory results). Reasons for the development of unihemispheric slumber are likely that it enables the sleeping animal to receive stimuli—threats, for case—from its environment, and that information technology enables the animal to fly or periodically surface to breathe when immersed in water. Just NREM sleep exists unihemispherically, and in that location seems to exist a continuum in unihemispheric sleep regarding the differences in the hemispheres: in animals exhibiting unihemispheric slumber, conditions range from one hemisphere beingness in deep sleep with the other hemisphere being awake to i hemisphere sleeping lightly with the other hemisphere beingness awake. If one hemisphere is selectively deprived of sleep in an fauna exhibiting unihemispheric slumber (one hemisphere is allowed to sleep freely but the other is awoken whenever information technology falls asleep), the amount of deep sleep volition selectively increase in the hemisphere that was deprived of sleep when both hemispheres are allowed to sleep freely.

The neurobiological background for unihemispheric sleep is still unclear. In experiments on cats in which the connexion between the left and the right halves of the brain stalk has been severed, the brain hemispheres show periods of a desynchronized EEG, during which the two hemispheres can sleep independently of each other.[83] In these cats, the state where one hemisphere slept NREM and the other was awake, as well as one hemisphere sleeping NREM with the other country sleeping REM were observed. The cats were never seen to slumber REM sleep with one hemisphere while the other hemisphere was awake. This is in accordance with the fact that REM slumber, as far equally is currently known, does not occur unihemispherically.

The fact that unihemispheric slumber exists has been used as an argument for the necessity of sleep.[84] Information technology appears that no animal has adult an ability to go without slumber altogether.

Hibernation [edit]

Animals that hide are in a state of torpor, differing from slumber. Hibernation markedly reduces the need for sleep, but does non remove information technology. Some hibernating animals end their hibernation a couple of times during the winter and so that they can slumber.[47] Hibernating animals waking up from hibernation frequently go into rebound slumber considering of lack of sleep during the hibernation period. They are definitely well-rested and are conserving free energy during hibernation, but need sleep for something else.[47]

References [edit]

  1. ^ Rechtschaffen A, Kales A (1968). A Manual of Standardised Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System of Sleep Stages of Human Subjects. Public Health Service. Washington: Government Printing Office.
  2. ^ Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson Jr A, Quan S (2007). AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specification. American Association of Sleep Medicine.
  3. ^ Meddis R (August 1975). "On the role of sleep". Animal Behaviour. 23 (3): 676–91. doi:x.1016/0003-3472(75)90144-X. PMID 169715. S2CID 11626959.
  4. ^ a b Nicolau MC, Akaârir M, Gamundí A, González J, Rial RV (November 2000). "Why we sleep: the evolutionary pathway to the mammalian slumber". Progress in Neurobiology. 62 (iv): 379–406. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(00)00013-7. PMID 10856610. S2CID 34642661.
  5. ^ Cirelli C, Tononi Chiliad (26 August 2008). "Is sleep essential?". PLOS Biology. Public Library of Science. six (eight): e216. doi:ten.1371/journal.pbio.0060216. PMC2525690. PMID 18752355. ... it would seem that searching for a core office of sleep, peculiarly at the cellular level, remains a worthwhile practise
  6. ^ Arnold, Carrie (2017). "Jellyfish defenseless snoozing give clues to origin of slumber". Nature News. doi:x.1038/nature.2017.22654 – via www.nature.com.
  7. ^ A sleep-like state in Hydra unravels conserved sleep mechanisms during the evolutionary evolution of the central nervous organisation - Science
  8. ^ Anafi, Ron C.; Kayser, Matthew S.; Raizen, David M. (February 2019). "Exploring phylogeny to find the part of slumber". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 20 (two): 109–116. doi:ten.1038/s41583-018-0098-9. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 30573905. S2CID 56575839.
  9. ^ Raizen DM, Zimmerman JE, Maycock MH, et al. (January 2008). "Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like land". Nature. 451 (7178): 569–72. Bibcode:2008Natur.451..569R. doi:10.1038/nature06535. PMID 18185515. S2CID 4342966.
  10. ^ "Practise insects sleep?". The Directly Dope. June 1999. Retrieved 11 Baronial 2013.
  11. ^ Shaw, Paul; Cirelli, C.; Greenspan, R.; Totoni, G. (2000). "Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster". Scientific discipline. 287 (5459): 1834–1837. Bibcode:2000Sci...287.1834S. doi:x.1126/scientific discipline.287.5459.1834. PMID 10710313.
  12. ^ Helfrich-Förster, Charlotte (2018-01-07). "Sleep in Insects". Annual Review of Entomology. Almanac Reviews. 63 (i): 69–86. doi:10.1146/annurev-ento-020117-043201. ISSN 0066-4170. PMID 28938081.
  13. ^ Huber, R; Hill, SL; Holladay, C; Biesiadecki, M; Tononi, K; Cirelli, C (June 2004). "Sleep homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster". Sleep. 27 (4): 628–39. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.4.628. PMID 15282997.
  14. ^ Tobler I, Neuner-Jehle Chiliad (Dec 1992). "24-h variation of vigilance in the cockroach Blaberus giganteus". Journal of Sleep Research. one (4): 231–239. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00044.ten. PMID 10607056. S2CID 8886069.
  15. ^ Ramón, F; Hernández-Falcón, J; Nguyen, B; Bullock, Thursday (Baronial 2004). "Slow wave sleep in crayfish". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United states of america of America. 101 (32): 11857–61. Bibcode:2004PNAS..10111857R. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402015101. PMC511064. PMID 15286285.
  16. ^ Riley, Alex. "Bees learn while they sleep, and that means they might dream". BBC News.
  17. ^ Park, Peter J. (Dec 2011). "Do Fish Slumber?: Fascinating Answers to Questions well-nigh Fishes. Animate being Q&A: Fascinating Answers to Questions about Animals. By Judith South. Weis. New Brunswick (New Jersey): Rutgers University Press...". The Quarterly Review of Biology (Review). 86 (4): 360–361. doi:ten.1086/662448. ISSN 0033-5770.
  18. ^ Reebs, S. (1992) Sleep, inactivity and circadian rhythms in fish. pp. 127–135 in: Ali, Thou.A. (ed.), Rhythms in Fish, New York: Plenum Press.
  19. ^ Kavanau JL (July 1998). "Vertebrates that never sleep: implications for sleep'southward basic role". Encephalon Inquiry Bulletin. 46 (4): 269–79. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00018-5. PMID 9671258. S2CID 6626805.
  20. ^ Parzefall J (1993). "Behavioural ecology of cave-dwelling fish". In Pitcher TJ (ed.). The Behaviour of Teleost Fish. London: Chapman & Hall. pp. 573–606. doi:x.1007/978-1-4684-8261-4_17. ISBN978-i-4684-8261-4.
  21. ^ Zhdanova, I.5.; Wang, Due south.Y.; Leclair, O.U.; Danilova, N.P.; et al. (2001). "Melatonin promotes sleep-similar state in zebrafish". Brain Research. Elsevier BV. 903 (1–2): 263–268. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02444-1. ISSN 0006-8993. PMID 11382414. S2CID 809510.
  22. ^ Yokogawa T, Marin W, Faraco J, Pézeron G, Appelbaum L, Zhang J, et al. (October 2007). "Label of Sleep in Zebrafish and Insomnia in Hypocretin Receptor Mutants". PLOS Biology. 5 (10): e277. doi:ten.1371/journal.pbio.0050277. PMC2020497. PMID 17941721. ; third party discussion of Yokogawa: Jones R (Oct 2007). "Let sleeping zebrafish prevarication: a new model for sleep studies". PLOS Biological science. v (10): e281. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050281. PMC2020498. PMID 20076649.
  23. ^ Shapiro, C.K.; Hepburn, H.R. (May 1976). "Sleep in a schooling fish, Tilapia mossambica". Physiology & Behavior. xvi (five): 613–5. doi:ten.1016/0031-9384(76)90222-five. PMID 972954. S2CID 41128895.
  24. ^ Peyrethon, J.; Dusan-Peyrethon, D. (1967). "Étude polygraphique du bike veille-sommeil d'un téléostéen (Tinca tinca)". Comptes Rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie. 161: 2533–2537.
  25. ^ Titkov, Eastward.South. (1976). "Characteristics of the daily periodicity of wakefulness and rest in the brown bullhead (Ictalurus nebulosus)". Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology. 12: 305–309.
  26. ^ Nelson, D.R.; Johnson, R.H. (1970). "Diel activity rhythms in the nocturnal, bottom-dwelling sharks Heterodontus francisci and Cephaloscyllium ventriosum". Copeia. 1970 (4): 732–739. doi:ten.2307/1442315. JSTOR 1442315.
  27. ^ Tauber, E.S., 1974, The phylogeny of slumber, pp. 133–172 in: Advances in sleep research, vol. 1 (E.D. Weitzman, ed.), Spectrum Publications, New York.
  28. ^ Weber, E (1961). "Über Ruhelagen von Fischen". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 18 (5): 517–533. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1961.tb00240.x.
  29. ^ Reebs, South.Thousand. (2002). "Plasticity of diel and circadian action rhythms in fish". Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries. 12 (4): 349–371. doi:x.1023/a:1025371804611. S2CID 33118836.
  30. ^ Low PS, Shank SS, Sejnowski TJ, Margoliash D (July 2008). "Mammalian-similar features of sleep structure in zebra finches". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.s.a. of America. 105 (26): 9081–six. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.9081L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0703452105. PMC2440357. PMID 18579776.
  31. ^ Leung LC, Wang GX, Madelaine R, Skariah G, Kawakami Thou, Deisseroth K, et al. (July 2019). "Neural signatures of sleep in zebrafish". Nature. 571 (7764): 198–204. Bibcode:2019Natur.571..198L. doi:ten.1038/s41586-019-1336-seven. PMC7081717. PMID 31292557.
  32. ^ Rial RV, Akaârir M, Gamundí A, Nicolau C, Garau C, Aparicio S, Tejada South, Gené 50, González J, De Vera LM, Coenen AM, Barceló P, Esteban S (July 2010). "Evolution of wakefulness, sleep and hibernation: from reptiles to mammals". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 34 (8): 1144–lx. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.008. PMID 20109487. S2CID 41872887.
  33. ^ Flanigan WF (1973). "Slumber and wakefulness in iguanid lizards, Ctenosaura pectinata and Iguana iguana". Brain, Beliefs and Evolution. 8 (6): 401–36. doi:ten.1159/000124366. PMID 4802023.
  34. ^ Shein-Idelson, Yard.; Ondracek, J. K.; Liaw, H.-P.; Reiter, Southward.; Laurent, One thousand. (Apr 2016). "Dull waves, abrupt waves, ripples, and REM in sleeping dragons". Science. 352 (6285): 590–5. Bibcode:2016Sci...352..590S. doi:ten.1126/science.aaf3621. PMID 27126045. S2CID 6604923.
  35. ^ Rattenborg, NC (March 2006). "Evolution of ho-hum-moving ridge sleep and palliopallial connectivity in mammals and birds: a hypothesis". Brain Inquiry Bulletin. 69 (1): 20–9. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.11.002. PMID 16464681. S2CID 19190804.
  36. ^ Lee Kavanau, J (Dec 2002). "REM and NREM slumber as natural accompaniments of the evolution of warm-bloodedness". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 26 (8): 889–906. doi:x.1016/s0149-7634(02)00088-x. PMID 12667495. S2CID 53299731.
  37. ^ Martinez-Gonzalez, Dolores; John A. Lesku; Niels C. Rattenborg (19 March 2008). "Increased EEG spectral ability density during slumber following brusque-term sleep deprivation in pigeons (Columba livia): evidence for avian sleep homeostasis". Journal of Sleep Research. 17 (two): 140–53. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00636.ten. PMID 18321247. S2CID 12759314. Interestingly, the independent evolution of like slumber states in birds and mammals might be related to the fact that each grouping besides independently evolved big brains capable of performing complex cognitive processes.
    • "'Power Napping' In Pigeons". ScienceDaily (Printing release). March half dozen, 2008.
  38. ^ Roth, TC 2; Lesku, JA; Amlander, CJ; Lima, SL (December 2006). "A phylogenetic analysis of the correlates of sleep in birds". Journal of Slumber Research. fifteen (4): 395–402. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00559.x. PMID 17118096. S2CID 15990945.
  39. ^ Rattenborg, NC; Amlaner, CJ; Lima, SL (December 2000). "Behavioral, neurophysiological and evolutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 24 (viii): 817–42. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00039-7. PMID 11118608. S2CID 7592942.
  40. ^ Rattenborg, NC; Amlaner, CJ; Lima, SL (2001). "Unilateral heart closure and interhemispheric EEG disproportion during sleep in the pigeon (Columba livia)". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 58 (6): 323–32. doi:10.1159/000057573. PMID 12016351. S2CID 45261403.
  41. ^ Mascetti, GG; Rugger, G; Vallortigara, G; Bobbo, D (January 2007). "Monocular-unihemispheric sleep and visual discrimination learning in the domestic chick". Experimental Brain Enquiry. 176 (1): seventy–84. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0595-3. PMID 16874518. S2CID 14246719.
  42. ^ Rattenborg, NC; Lima, SL; Amlaner, CJ (November 1999). "Facultative command of avian unihemispheric sleep under the risk of predation". Behavioural Brain Research. 105 (2): 163–72. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00070-4. PMID 10563490. S2CID 8570743.
  43. ^ Siegel Jerome 1000 (April 2008). "Do all animals sleep?". Trends in Neurosciences. 31 (4): 208–13. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.02.001. PMC8765194. PMID 18328577. S2CID 6614359.
  44. ^ Capellini I, Nunn CL, McNamara P, Preston BT, Barton RA (October 2008). "Energetic constraints, not predation, influence the evolution of slumber patterning in mammals". Functional Environmental. 22 (5): 847–853. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01449.x. PMC2860325. PMID 20428321.
  45. ^ "Slumber Syllabus. B. The Phylogeny of Sleep". Sleep Research Gild, Pedagogy Committee. Archived from the original on 2005-03-eighteen. Retrieved 26 September 2010.
  46. ^ "Function of Slumber.". Scribd.com. Retrieved on 1 December 2011.
  47. ^ a b c Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM (July 1991). "Warming upwards for sleep? Basis squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation" (PDF). Neuroscience Letters. 128 (2): 265–eight. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y. PMID 1945046. S2CID 13802495. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-06-06. Retrieved 2019-08-18 .
  48. ^ Challet Etienne (December 2007). "Minireview: Entrainment of the suprachiasmatic clockwork in diurnal and nocturnal mammals". Endocrinology. 148 (12): 5648–55. doi:10.1210/en.2007-0804. PMID 17901231.
  49. ^ Fred Due west. Turek & Charles A. Czeisler (1999). "Role of Melatonin in the Regulation of Sleep", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 181–195.
  50. ^ David R. Weaver (1999), "Melatonin and Circadian Rhythmicity in Vertebrates: Physiological Roles and Pharmacological Effects", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Cyclic Rhythms, pp. 197–262.
  51. ^ Eve Van Cauter & Karine Spiegel (1999). "Cyclic and Sleep Command of Hormonal Secretions", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 397–425.
  52. ^ Thomas A. Wehr (1999). "The Touch of Changes in Nightlength (Scotoperiod) on Human Sleep", in Turek & Zee (eds.), Regulation of Sleep and Circadian Rhythms, pp. 263–285.
  53. ^ Rattenborg, N.C.; Voirin, B.; Vyssotski, A.L.; Kays, R.W.; Spoelstra, Thou.; Kuemmeth, F.; Heidrich, W.; Wikelski, M. (August 2008). "Sleeping outside the box: electroencephalographic measures of sleep in sloths inhabiting a rainforest". Biological science Letters. iv (iv): 402–5. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0203. PMC2610152. PMID 18482903.
  54. ^ a b c Siegel, Jerome 1000. (October 2005). "Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep" (PDF). Nature. Nature Portfolio. 437 (7063): 1264–71. Bibcode:2005Natur.437.1264S. doi:10.1038/nature04285. PMC8760626. PMID 16251951. S2CID 234089. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2007-08-23. Retrieved 2008-01-04 .
  55. ^ a b Wong, Sam (March 2017). "Elephants slumber for just two hours a day – the to the lowest degree of any mammal". NewScientist.
  56. ^ Opp MR (January 2009). "Sleeping to fuel the immune system: mammalian sleep and resistance to parasites". BMC Evolutionary Biology. BioMed Central Ltd. ix: viii. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-8. PMC2633283. PMID 19134176.
  57. ^ The Sleep Research Club (1997). "Sleep Syllabus". Basics of Slumber Behavior. U.s.: WebSciences International and Sleep Research Guild. Archived from the original on 2005-03-18. Retrieved 2008-04-16 .
  58. ^ Faraco, Juliette (1 Baronial 2000). "Re: Are in that location animals who don't slumber or that sleep very little?". MadSci Network: Zoology . Retrieved 25 January 2008.
  59. ^ Insomnia Mania: Newborn Mammals Don't Sleep for a Month. LiveScience.com
  60. ^ a b c Hecker, Bruce (ii February 1998). "How practice Whales and Dolphins Sleep without Drowning?". Scientific American. mirror Archived 2017-02-16 at the Wayback Car
  61. ^ Britt, Robert (29 June 2005). "Insomnia Mania: Newborn Mammals Don't Slumber for a Month". Live Scientific discipline.
  62. ^ "How Horses Sleep Pt. ii – Power Naps". Archived from the original on 2007-09-27.
  63. ^ a b Holland, Jennifer S. "forty Winks?", National Geographic Vol. 220, No. 1, July 2011.
  64. ^ a b Datta South (Nov 2000). "Abstention task training potentiates phasic pontine-moving ridge density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity". The Journal of Neuroscience. xx (22): 8607–13. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-08607.2000. PMC6773158. PMID 11069969.
  65. ^ Kudrimoti HS, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL (May 1999). "Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics". The Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (10): 4090–101. doi:ten.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04090.1999. PMC6782694. PMID 10234037.
  66. ^ Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council (2003). Guidelines for the Intendance and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. The National Academies Press. p. 121. doi:10.17226/10732. ISBN978-0-309-08903-half dozen. PMID 20669478. Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually sepsis and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983).
  67. ^ Gümüştekín K, Seven B, Karabulut N, Aktaş O, Gürsan N, Aslan South, Keleş Thousand, Varoglu Eastward, Dane S (November 2004). "Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats". The International Periodical of Neuroscience. 114 (11): 1433–42. doi:10.1080/00207450490509168. PMID 15636354. S2CID 30346608.
  68. ^ Zager A, Andersen ML, Ruiz FS, Antunes IB, Tufik S (July 2007). "Furnishings of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats". American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 293 (one): R504-nine. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00105.2007. PMID 17409265.
  69. ^ Dalanon, Junhel; Chikahisa, Sachiko; Shiuchi, Tetsuya; Shimizu, Noriyuki; Chavan, Parimal; Suzuki, Yoshitaka; Okura, Kazuo; Séi, Hiroyoshi; Matsuka, Yoshizo (July 2021). "Pain sensitivity increases with slumber disturbance under predictable chronic mild stress in mice". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 14231. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1114231D. doi:x.1038/s41598-021-93560-7. PMC8271003. PMID 34244555.
  70. ^ Peres, Judy (14 March 2012) A practiced reason to get your zzz's Chicago Tribune Health, retrieved 26 March 2014
  71. ^ Siegel, JM; Manger, PR; Nienhuis, R; Fahringer, HM; Pettigrew, JD (May 1996). "The echidna Tachyglossus aculeatus combines REM and non-REM aspects in a single sleep state: implications for the evolution of sleep". The Periodical of Neuroscience. 16 (x): 3500–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-10-03500.1996. PMC6579141. PMID 8627382.
  72. ^ Siegel, JM; Manger, PR; Nienhuis, R; Fahringer, HM; Pettigrew, JD (July 1998). "Monotremes and the development of rapid eye motility sleep". Philosophical Transactions of the Majestic Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 353 (1372): 1147–57. doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0272. PMC1692309. PMID 9720111.
  73. ^ Siegel, J.M.; P.R. Manger; R. Nienhuis; H.M. Fahringer; T. Shalita; J.D. Pettigrew (June 1999). "Slumber in the platypus". Neuroscience. Elsevier. 91 (one): 391–400. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00588-0. PMC8760620. PMID 10336087. S2CID 18766417.
  74. ^ "Seals Slumber with Only Half of Their Brain at a Time". Oceana.org. 12 March 2013.
  75. ^ Lapierre JL, Kosenko PO, Lyamin OI, Kodama T, Mukhametov LM, Siegel JM (October 2007). "Cortical acetylcholine release is lateralized during asymmetrical slow-moving ridge sleep in northern fur seals". The Periodical of Neuroscience. 27 (44): 11999–2006. doi:ten.1523/JNEUROSCI.2968-07.2007. PMC6673386. PMID 17978041.
  76. ^ "Report Seals Sleep with Half Their Brain". upi.com. nineteen February 2013.
  77. ^ Amanda Schaffer (27 May 2007). "Why do we Sleep?". Slate.com. Retrieved 23 August 2008.
  78. ^ Serafetinides, EA; Shurley, JT; Brooks, RE (1972). "Electroencephalogram of the pilot whale, Globicephala scammoni, in wakefulness and slumber: lateralization aspects". Int J Psychobiol. 2: 129–135.
  79. ^ Ridgway, SH; Harrison, RJ; Joyce, PL (Feb 1975). "Sleep and cardiac rhythm in the greyness seal". Scientific discipline. 187 (4176): 553–5. Bibcode:1975Sci...187..553R. doi:10.1126/science.163484. PMID 163484.
  80. ^ Sekiguchi Y, Arai M, Kohshima S (June 2006). "Slumber behaviour: sleep in continuously active dolphins". Nature. 441 (7096): E9-10, discussion E11. Bibcode:2006Natur.441E...9S. doi:ten.1038/nature04898. PMID 16791150. S2CID 4406032.
  81. ^ Miller PJ, Aoki K, Rendell LE, Amano G (Jan 2008). "Stereotypical resting behavior of the sperm whale". Current Biology. eighteen (ane): R21-3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.eleven.003. PMID 18177706. S2CID 10587736.
  82. ^ Mukhametov LM, Supin AY, Polyakova IG (October 1977). "Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic slumber patterns in dolphins". Brain Research. 134 (3): 581–four. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90835-half-dozen. PMID 902119. S2CID 31725807.
  83. ^ Michel, F.; Roffwarg, H.P. (Feb 1967). "Chronic separate brain stem preparation: effect on the sleep-waking cycle". Experientia (in French). Birkhäuser. 23 (2): 126–8. doi:10.1007/BF02135958. PMID 6032104. S2CID 37925278.
  84. ^ Cirelli, C; M Tononi (August 2008). "Is sleep essential?". PLOS Biology. 6 (viii): e216. doi:x.1371/journal.pbio.0060216. PMC2525690. PMID 18752355.

External links [edit]

  • Sleep in fish
  • Dog dreams

Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sleep_in_animals

Posted by: masonpate1995.blogspot.com

0 Response to "Does Animal Sleep In Any Particular Direction"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel